MovieChat Forums > The Interview (1998) Discussion > somebody knew what they were doing

somebody knew what they were doing


This film is particularly good if you've ever done any reading on the subject of false confessions and other aspects of police interrogations, such as fixing on a single theory and then getting tunnel vision re the evidence; making one critical prejudgment and then shaping every subsequent decision to confirm that first decision; lying during interrogations, to make the suspect (or interviewee) believe the case against him is much stronger than it is; and so on. Whoever advised on this film _really_ knew what he/she was doing.

(I'm exempting certain obvious flaws such as the fact that nobody seems particularly interested in going out to where the burnt lean-to is supposed to be, look for charred remains, etc.)

Also on target is what Fleming said about the tendency of police to let public pressure over unsolved crimes get in the way of investigative thoroughness and objectivity. That is absolutely true--and further evidence that it occurred here comes in the fact that they keep repeating the fact that there is no link between the disappearances and not even any bodies, if I recall correctly. That's the big fact that those convinced of Fleming's guilt are missing--it's nowhere near certain that a crime has even been committed at all. The whole film seems to be more about our need to gather up unexplained occurrences and put a structure or meaning to them.

It's also true that the occasional interviewee under threat or pressure will make a false confession that is deliberately intended to mislead police, who will then be confounded and/or embarrassed by the discovery that they've been duped (or so the suspect thinks). Not a good idea, but I think most people would be surprised what they would do when under that kind of duress, when told people had identified them at the scene, that fingerprints matched, etc.

reply

Almost three years on, but anyway. . . :P

I couldn't agree with you more. I watched it for the first time a couple of months ago and believed that he was most likely innocent. Today I watched it again from the officer's points of view and tried to see him as guilty. And even after studying his facial expressions, his tone of voice and basically every move he made I still think he's innocent. I think you can almost even see him making up the story as he begins telling it before he really gets into it and it starts to flow.
And if you listen he doesn't tell them anything they haven't already told him. They just wanted to believe he did it so badly that they couldn't think otherwise.

Brilliant acting, direction and script! I love this film!

---
Human beings are a disease, a cancer of this planet, you are a plague... and we are the cure.

reply

>> They just wanted to believe he did it so badly that they couldn't think otherwise. <<

Exactly it. For a very large percentage of interrogators and prosecutors (and maybe this is just a broad human quality anyway) their intent to find a perpetrator overrides everything else. Even procedurally, once that initial decision is made that "this is the guy," that's really the end of any true investigation, and every decision from that point onward--even if it's just after the arrest, or even _before_ the arrest in some cases--is made to justify that initial decision. Particularly for a prosecutor, you don't build your reputation by being fair or pursuing justice; you build that reputation and get promoted based on convictions, period.

You're also SO right about how he doesn't really give the police much, if anything they, didn't already know. What the general public doesn't understand is that for an accused person under duress, everything moves quickly away from establishing an account of events that is actually true to establishing an account that accomplishes the purpose of relieving the pressure or pain or that results in release (or the promise of release). Even in situations where the interrogators don't intend to be deceptive, and especially in situations where there are multiple interrogators, the accused will pick up various aspects of the story that the interrogator doesn't even realize he's mentioning, or the accused will pick them up by implication. Many times the accused will try this version and that one until he hits on something that happens to be what the interrogators know to be true (about the crime scene, circumstantial evidence, whatever), and/or on something that seems at first glance to be specific to the crime but really isn't: He knew the second victim had brown hair!...but even from a pure guess, that has probably a 70% chance (or so) of being true, especially when you expand the definition of "brown" to include anything darker than light blonde and not completely grey or red or jet-black. Or, he knew the second victim was a prostitute, and he even knew the part of town where she worked!...but one interrogator had already let it slip that these were "hooker murders," and there's only one area of town where they commonly work. And so forth.

The relative ease of producing false confessions and of convicting innocent people is much scarier than almost anybody in the general population realizes. The same is true for eyewitness testimony and the kinds of things that convince a jury, the perception most people have of their ability to "spot a liar" (they can't, but most people are convinced they can, which is even worse), and so forth. It's also a fascinating--but chilling--area of social psychology.

reply

[deleted]

[deleted]

[deleted]